Communication strategies (seven studies; range 174 to 5,500 participants)
Strength of evidence was considered low or insufficient for all findings. Risk of bias was low for two studies and moderate for five.
Framing versus narratives: Loss-framed messages (such as emphasising what was lost by taking an action or making a choice) used with narratives were more persuasive than loss-framed messages in conjunction with statistical information alone or gain-framed messages in conjunction with either narratives or statistical information (one trial).
Framing versus targeting: The loss-framed message used in combination with non-targeting (a broader appeal either culturally or societally, such as a collectivist appeal) was most persuasive relative to any other combination of framing (conveying the same message in alternative ways) and targeting (designed for specific subgroups based on group membership or characteristics) (two trials).
Targeting versus tailoring: Evidence that compared targeting with tailoring (communication designed for an individual based on information from the individual) was mixed. There were no significant differences in outcomes between those receiving the targeted or tailored version of a communication intervention in two studies. In a third study, the targeted version was associated with a greater likelihood of self-reported screening compared with the tailored version. There were no statistically significant differences between targeting plus tailoring versus tailoring alone (one trial).
Dissemination strategies (38 studies; range 114 to 3,293 participants)
Multi-component strategies targeting clinicians that address a combination of reach, ability, or motivation were more effective than one strategy alone for affecting clinician behaviours, notably guideline adherence (seven trials; moderate strength of evidence). The strength of evidence was low or insufficient for most comparisons related to clinical outcomes and knowledge for clinicians.
Evidence on the benefit of reach, ability, motivation, or multi-component approaches for patients focused on changing health-related decisions and behaviours was inconsistent (12 trials; insufficient strength of evidence). Evidence was insufficient for determining the benefit of reach, ability, motivation, or multi-component approaches for patients for clinical outcomes (two trials; low or insufficient strength of evidence) and knowledge outcomes (three trials; insufficient strength of evidence).
Evidence was inconsistent on the benefit of reach, ability, motivation, or multi-component strategies that targeted both providers and patients for health-related decisions and behaviours (six trials) or clinical outcomes (one trial). Strength of evidence was insufficient for strategies that target both providers and patients.
Communicating uncertainty (nine studies; range 120 to 2,944 participants)
Six RCTs, two quasi-RCTs and one non-controlled trial were included.
Precision: Studies on communicating precision of evidence found mixed effects of presenting numeric risks as point estimates versus 95% confidence intervals. Only a single small study examined the effects of changing the format in which 95% confidence intervals were presented (numeric versus graphical) on perceived risk of colon cancer (insufficient strength of evidence). A single small study examined the effects of using clean versus blurry bar graphs to convey information about uncertainty (insufficient strength of evidence). One study found that consumers' cholesterol medication choice was better when they received non-numeric advice or factual information which encouraged choice of a medication with direct evidence of benefit compared with usual care (one study, low strength of evidence).
Directness: Medication choice was better among participants who received non-numeric advice or factual information which encouraged consumers to choose a drug with greater net benefit than in patients who received usual care (one study; low strength of evidence). Receiving additional nonnumeric information about benefits had little effect on refusals of cancer screening tests, but receiving more non-numeric information on harms significantly increased test refusals and significantly reduced decision satisfaction (one study; low strength of evidence).
Net benefit: Providing men with prostate cancer screening information alone or framed in the context of information about other more beneficial screening services significantly increased prostate cancer knowledge (one study; low strength of evidence) compared with usual care. However, providing prostate cancer screening information alone versus framed in the broader context of more beneficial services had differential effects on patient involvement and screening (two studies; insufficient strength of evidence).
Strength of recommendations: Only one small study provided insufficient evidence on the effectiveness of providing different ways of wording recommendations to convey strong or weak recommendations for care.